Cultural Affiliation and Language Methodological Problems in Cross-Cultural Experimental Philosophy
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.14394/filnau.2018.0003Keywords:
experimental philosophy, methodology of cross-cultural studies, cultural affiliation, content validity, philosophical intuitionsAbstract
The aim of this paper is to analyze methodological problems of cross-cultural research in experimental philosophy. By studying five research projects, representing two distinct approaches to the examination of cross-cultural differences in philosophical intuitions, I point out the difficulties related to the content validity that appear in the choice of some cultural affiliation indicators. I criticize various indicators of cultural affiliation that are used in experimental philosophy (selfidentification, language) and suggest which indicators, and how, should be chosen in that kind of research. I also argue that some problems related to bilingualism, translation, and the possible influence of language on the owned or reported philosophical intuitions may be impossible to transcend in cross-cultural research in experimental philosophy. It may be impossible to assess the differences between cultural philosophical intuitions.
References
Abarbanell L., Hauser M. (2010), Mayan Morality. An Exploration of Permissible Harms, "Cognition" 115(2), 207-224.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.007
Andow J. (2016), Thin, Fine and with Sensitivity. A Metamethodology of Intuitions, "Review of Philosophy and Psychology" 7(1), 105-125. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-015-0247-2
Auer P. (1998), Code-Switching in Conversation, London: Routledge.
Bar-On D. (1994), Conceptual Relativism and Translation [w:] Language, Mind and Epistemology, J. Hintikka (ed.), Dordrecht: Springer, 145-170.
Cappelen H. (2012), Philosophy without Intuitions, Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199644865.001.0001
Cappelen H. (2014), X-Phi without Intuitions [w:] Intuitions, A. Booth, D. Rowbottom (eds.), Oxford: Oxford University Press, 269-286.
Davis R. E., Couper M. P., Janz N. K., Caldwell C. H., Resnicow K. (2010), Interviewer Effects in Public Health Surveys, "Health Education Research" 25(1), 14-26. https://doi.org/10.1093/her/cyp046
Deutsch M. (2010), Intuitions, Counter-examples, and Experimental Philosophy, "Review of Philosophy and Psychology" 1(3), 447-460. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-010-0033-0
Dretske F. I. (1970), Epistemic Operators, "The Journal of Philosophy" 67(24), 1007-1023. https://doi.org/10.2307/2024710
Durkheim E. (1912/1990), Elementarne formy życia religijnego. System totemiczny w Australii, tłum. A. Zadrożyńska, Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.
Everett D. (2005), Cultural Constraints on Grammar and Cognition in Piraha. Another Look at the Design Features of Human Language, "Current Anthropology" 46(4), 621-646. https://doi.org/10.1086/431525
Górski T. (2005), Tłumacz jako źródło nieprzetłumaczalności, "Rocznik Przekładoznawczy" 1, 85-94. https://doi.org/10.12775/RP.2005.007
Habermas J. (1981/2002), Teoria działania komunikacyjnego, t. II, Przyczynek do krytyki rozumu funkcjonalnego, tłum. A. M. Kaniowski, Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.
Hall L., Johansson P., Strandberg T. (2012), Lifting the Veil of Morality. Choice Blindness and Attitude Reversals on a Self-Transforming Survey, "PLoS ONE" 7(9), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045457
Holbraad M. (2009), Ontography and Alterity. Defining Anthropological Truth, "Social Analysis" 53(2), 80-93. https://doi.org/10.3167/sa.2009.530205
Kauppinen A. (2007), The Rise and Fall of Experimental Philosophy, "Philosophical Explorations" 10(2), 95-118.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13869790701305871
Kerr E. T. (2015), Epistemological Experiments and Empirical Philosophy in Cross-Cultural Contexts, "Asia Research Institute Working Paper Series" 233, 1-27.
Kim M., Yuan Y. (2015), No Cross-Cultural Differences in the Gettier Car Case Intuition. A Replication Study of Weinberg et al. 2001, "Episteme" 12(3), 355-361. https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2015.17
Kim M., Yuan Y. (2016), Cross-Cultural Universality in Knowledge Attributions, goo.gl/6YR73E.
Kurzban, R. (2012), Why Everyone (Else) is a Hypocrite. Evolution and the Modular Mind, Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Kuś K., Maćkiewicz B. (2016), Z rozmysłem, ale nie specjalnie. O językowej wrażliwości filozofii eksperymentalnej, "Filozofia Nauki" 24(3) [95], 73-101.
Lee K., Olson D. R., Torrance N. (1999), Chinese Children's Understanding of False Beliefs. The Role of Language, "Journal of Child Language" 26(1), 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000998003626
Liu D., Wellman H. M., Tardif T., Sabbagh M. A. (2008), Theory of Mind Development in Chinese Children. A Meta-analysis of False-Belief Understanding across Cultures and Languages, "Developmental Psychology" 44(2), 523-531. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.523
Lucy J. A. (1997), Linguistic Relativity, "Annual Review of Anthropology" 26(1), 291-312. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.26.1.291
Ludwig K. (2007), The Epistemology of Thought Experiments. First Person versus Third Person Approaches, " Midwest Studies in Philosophy" 31(1), 128-159. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-4975.2007.00160.x
Machery E., Deutsch M., Mallon R., Nichols S., Sytsma J., Stich S. (2010), Semantic Intuitions. Reply to Lam, "Cognition" 117(3), 361-366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.016
Machery E., Stich S., Rose D., Chatterjee A., Karasawa K., Struchiner N., Sirker S., Usui N., Hashimoto T. (2015), Gettier across Cultures, "Noûs" 51(3), 645-664. https://doi.org/10.1111/nous.12110
Maridaki-Kassotaki K., Lewis C., Freeman N. H. (2003), Lexical Choice Can Lead to Problems. What False-Belief Tests Tell Us about Greek Alternative Verbs of Agency, "Journal of Child Language", 30(1), 145-164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000902005421
Michna E. (2004), Etyczny wymiar tożsamości etnicznej w sytuacji zmiany identyfikacji narodowej [w:] Etyczny wymiar tożsamości kulturowej, M. Flis (red.), Kraków: Nomos, 25-45.
Myers-Scotton C. (1993), Social Motivations for Codeswitching. Evidence from Africa, Oxford: Clarendon.
Nagel J., San Juan V., Mar R. A. (2013), Lay Denial of Knowledge for Justified True Beliefs, "Cognition" 129(3), 652-661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.008
Nisbett R. E., Wilson T. D. (1977), Telling More than We Can Know. Verbal Reports on Mental Processes, "Psychological Review" 84(3), 231-259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231
Nisbett R. E., Peng K., Choi I., Norenzayan A. (2001), Culture and Systems of Thought. Holistic versus Analytic Cognition, "Psychological Review", 108(2), 291-310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291
Pinker S. (1995), The Language Instinct. How the Mind Creates Language, New York: Harper Perennial.
Roberson D., Davies I., Davidoff J. (2000), Color Categories Are not Universal. Replications and New Evidence from a Stone-Age Culture, "Journal of Experimental Psychology: General" 129(3), 369-398. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.369
Romaniszyn K. (2004), Konsumuję więc jestem? [w:] Etyczny wymiar tożsamości kulturowej, M. Flis (red.), Kraków: Nomos, 37-48.
Schwarz N. (1999), Self Reports. How the Questions Shape the Answers, "American Psychologist" 54(2), 93-105. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.2.93
Seyedsayamdost H. (2015), On Normativity and Epistemic Intuitions. Failure of Replication, "Episteme" 12(1), 95-116. https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2014.27
Shatz M., Diesendruck G., Martinez-Beck I., Akar D. (2003), The Influence of Language and Socioeconomic Status on Children's Understanding of False Belief, "Developmental Psychology" 39(4) 717-729. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.717
Starmans C., Friedman O. (2012), The Folk Conception of Knowledge, "Cognition" 124(3), 272-283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.017
Strack F., Martin L., Schwarz N. (1988), Priming and Communication. Social Determinants of Information Use in Judgments of Life Satisfaction, "European Journal of Social Psychology" 18(5), 429-442. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420180505
Sztompka P. (2002), Socjologia. Analiza społeczeństwa, Kraków: Znak.
Tambor J. (2010), Kulturowe wyznaczniki tożsamości. Tożsamość mieszkańców województwa śląskiego, https://goo.gl/RrGtDL.
Tardif T., Wellman H. M., Cheung K. M. (2004), False Belief Understanding in Cantonese-Speaking Children, "Journal of Child Language" 31(4), 779-800. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000904006580
Turri J. (2013), A Conspicuous Art. Putting Gettier to the Test, "Philosophers' Imprint" 13(10), 1-16.
Turri J., Buckwalter W., Blouw P. (2015), Knowledge and Luck, "Psychonomic Bulletin & Review" 22(2), 378-390. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0683-5
Von Hippel W., Trivers R. (2011), The Evolution and Psychology of Self-Deception, "Behavioral and Brain Sciences" 34(01), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10003018 https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10001354
Williamson T. (2016), Philosophical Criticisms of Experimental Philosophy [w:] A Companion to Experimental Philosophy, J. Sytsma, W. Buckwalter (eds.), Chichester: Wiley, 22-26.
Weinberg J. M., Nichols S., Stich S. (2001), Normativity and Epistemic Intuitions, "Philosophical Topics" 29(1/2), 429-460. https://doi.org/10.5840/philtopics2001291/217
Woolard K. (2004), Codeswitching [w:] A Companion to Linguistic Anthropology, A. Duranti (ed.), Malden, MA: Blackwell, 73-94.